pubmed-article:21816487 | rdf:type | pubmed:Citation | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0681827 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:21816487 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0439655 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:21816487 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0936012 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:issue | 2 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:dateCreated | 2011-9-19 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:abstractText | Affective dysfunction is a defining schizotypy feature; yet the majority of studies examining affective dysfunction have largely relied on self-report of trait affect, which reflects only one dimension of emotional experience. Emerging research has explored a second dimension, state affect, using laboratory manipulation, with most finding that schizotypal participants report experiencing less positive/more negative affect than controls. This study expands upon this topic by examining patterns of state affect in psychometrically identified schizotypy through self-report and lexical expression in reaction to emotionally valenced photos. Overall, the schizotypy group reported less positive/more negative affect across affect induction conditions. Both schizotypy and control groups' affect ratings were similar following the unpleasant stimuli; but the schizotypy group's ratings remained significantly less positive/more negative than the control group following the pleasant stimuli. This pattern suggests that the schizotypy group experienced a deficit in emotional reactivity compared to controls in pleasant situations. The schizotypy group also used a higher percentage of negative words and a lower percentage of positive words in vocalized reactions during the pleasant, but not unpleasant, affect induction condition. These results reveal a specific pattern of "in-the-moment" affective dysfunction unique to pleasant situations that is consistent across both subjective experience and lexical expression. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:language | eng | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:journal | http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:citationSubset | IM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:status | MEDLINE | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:month | Sep | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:issn | 0165-1781 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:CohenAlex SAS | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:MinorKyle SKS | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:NajoliaGina... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:copyrightInfo | Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:issnType | Print | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:day | 30 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:volume | 189 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:owner | NLM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:authorsComplete | Y | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:pagination | 233-8 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:21816487... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:year | 2011 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:articleTitle | A laboratory study of affectivity in schizotypy: subjective and lexical analysis. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:affiliation | Department of Psychology, Louisana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA. gnajol1@tigers.lsu.edu | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:21816487 | pubmed:publicationType | Journal Article | lld:pubmed |