Source:http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/id/16812487
Switch to
Predicate | Object |
---|---|
rdf:type | |
lifeskim:mentions | |
pubmed:issue |
1
|
pubmed:dateCreated |
2010-6-29
|
pubmed:abstractText |
In Experiment 1, pigeons were trained to peck keys with equal food-reinforcement schedules in components that ended with either noncontingent or contingent transitions to a third component with a five-fold richer schedule. Response rates were higher in the initial component with contingent transitions, but resistance to prefeeding or extinction was not consistently greater. Experiment 2 also included noncontingent or contingent transitions to a signaled period of nonreinforcement. There was no effect of the contingency on transitions to nonreinforcement, but the difference in response rates maintained by contingent versus noncontingent transitions to the richer schedule was replicated. In addition, response rates were higher in components that preceded nonreinforcement than in components that preceded the richer schedule. However, resistance to extinction was greater for noncontingent transitions to the richer schedule than to nonreinforcement, implicating stimulus-reinforcer relations in the determination of resistance to change. Resistance to change was also somewhat greater for noncontingent than for contingent transitions to the richer schedule. The latter result, together with the results of Experiment 1 and related research, suggests that response-contingent reinforcement does not increase resistance to change.
|
pubmed:commentsCorrections |
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-13861909,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-14055953,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-16811560,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-16811687,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-16811733,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-16811752,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-16812187,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-16812218,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-16812312,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-3559458,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-528894,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-5323024,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-562919,
http://linkedlifedata.com/resource/pubmed/commentcorrection/16812487-5722410
|
pubmed:language |
eng
|
pubmed:journal | |
pubmed:status |
PubMed-not-MEDLINE
|
pubmed:month |
Jul
|
pubmed:issn |
0022-5002
|
pubmed:author | |
pubmed:issnType |
Print
|
pubmed:volume |
48
|
pubmed:owner |
NLM
|
pubmed:authorsComplete |
Y
|
pubmed:pagination |
17-33
|
pubmed:dateRevised |
2010-9-15
|
pubmed:year |
1987
|
pubmed:articleTitle |
Does contingent reinforcement strengthen operant behavior?
|
pubmed:publicationType |
Journal Article
|