Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:8826759rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0037313lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:8826759lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1280500lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:8826759lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0439609lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:issue7lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:dateCreated1996-11-8lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:abstractTextNormal sleepers were instructed either to fall asleep as quickly as they could or to fall asleep whenever they desired, under a high mental load (listening to John Philip Sousa marches) or a low mental load (listening to sleep-conducive new age music). Under low load, participants trying to fall asleep quickly did so faster than those attempting only to fall asleep whenever they desired. Under high load, however, and consistent with the ironic process theory of mental control (Wegner, D. M., 1994, Psychological Review, 101, 34-52), sleep onset latency was greater for participants attempting to fall asleep quickly than for those not attempting to do so.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:granthttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:monthJullld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:issn0005-7967lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:authorpubmed-author:WegnerD MDMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:authorpubmed-author:BowserRRlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:authorpubmed-author:AnsfieldM EMElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:issnTypePrintlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:volume34lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:pagination523-31lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:dateRevised2007-11-14lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:8826759-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:8826759-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:8826759-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:8826759-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:8826759-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:year1996lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:articleTitleIronic effects of sleep urgency.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:affiliationDepartment of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville 22903, USA. mea2z@virginia.edu; dwegner@virginia.edulld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:8826759pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S.lld:pubmed