Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:21547767rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0022417lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21547767lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0013987lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21547767lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0441472lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21547767lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1280500lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21547767lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0220825lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21547767lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0392756lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21547767lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0449450lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:issue4lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:dateCreated2011-5-6lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:abstractTextIn four experiments conducted on the world wide web, subjects evaluated the priority of policies presented separately or presented jointly in pairs, and/or reported their emotional responses to the problem that each policy addressed. Strength of emotional responses was more strongly related to priority when policies were presented separately than when they were presented jointly. We found evidence for one mechanism that could produce these results: joint presentation increases the evaluability of the policies, thus increasing the influence of cognitive evaluations of importance on priority judgements, and reducing the relative influence of emotional responses. We also found evidence that importance can affect emotional responses. We found no evidence for other mechanisms in which the emotions evoked by one item spread to the other item in joint presentation. The role of evaluability points to the applied value of evaluating policies in the context of alternatives.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:monthJunlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:issn1464-0600lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:authorpubmed-author:BaronJonathan...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:authorpubmed-author:RitovIlanaIlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:issnTypeElectroniclld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:volume25lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:pagination657-75lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21547767...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:year2011lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:articleTitleJoint presentation reduces the effect of emotion on evaluation of public actions.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:affiliationHebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21547767pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov'tlld:pubmed