Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:11817751rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0004268lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11817751lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0001762lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11817751lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1280500lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11817751lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0439656lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:issue12lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:dateCreated2002-1-30lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:abstractTextThe effect of voluntary attention on afterimage fragmentation was explored in two experiments. The afterimage, in the form of a 30 degrees-tilted star of David, was generated after prolonged steady fixation in the first experiment, and with a brief and intense flash in the second experiment. Subjects were instructed to select various target shapes in the afterimage for attention and, at the same time, observe what was visible or invisible. Verbal reports and manual responses to afterimage changes were analyzed. Attended shapes were found to disappear from awareness Faster than unattended ones (experiment 1), and complementary shapes were found to predominate visual awareness when one of the pair was selected for attention (experiment 2). Voluntary attention was also found to affect closure (filling-in of enclosed regions) and smoothing of line figures in afterimages.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:issn0301-0066lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:authorpubmed-author:WuL TLTlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:issnTypePrintlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:volume30lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:pagination1439-48lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:dateRevised2006-11-15lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11817751...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:year2001lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:articleTitleEffects of voluntary attention on structured afterimages.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:affiliationDepartment of Psychology, Grand Valley State Universty, Allendale, MI 49401, USA. loul@gvsu.edulld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11817751pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov'tlld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:11817751lld:pubmed