Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:9530834rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0029266lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:9530834lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0546816lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:9530834lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0013139lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:issue4lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:dateCreated1998-4-30lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:abstractTextIn arena experiments with the walking fruit fly, we found a remarkable persistence of orientation toward a landmark that disappeared during the fly's approach. The directional stability achieved by 'after-fixation' allows a fly to continue pursuit under natural conditions, where a selected target is frequently concealed by surrounding structures. The persistence of after-fixation was investigated in Buridan's paradigm, where a fly walks persistently back and forth between two inaccessible landmarks. Upon disappearance of a selected target, the flies maintained their intended course for more than 15 body lengths of approximately 2.5 mm in about 50% of the trials. About 13% even exceeded 75 body lengths. About 88% of the approaches clustered in equal portions around peaks at 2.4 s and 8.6 s. About 12% of the approaches persisted even longer. In contrast, a single peak at about 2.2 s is sufficient to describe the persistence of orientation in a random walk. The ability to pursue an invisible landmark is disturbed neither by a transient angular deviation from the course toward this landmark, when this target disappeared, nor by a distracting second landmark. Accordingly, after-fixation seems to require an internal representation of the direction toward the concealed target, and idiothetical course control to maintain this direction.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:monthAprlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:issn0340-7594lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:authorpubmed-author:StraussRRlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:authorpubmed-author:PichlerJJlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:issnTypePrintlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:volume182lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:pagination411-23lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:dateRevised2009-6-4lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9530834-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9530834-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9530834-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9530834-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9530834-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:year1998lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:articleTitlePersistence of orientation toward a temporarily invisible landmark in Drosophila melanogaster.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:affiliationMax-Planck-Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Tübingen, Germany. roland.strauss@tuebingen.mpg.delld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9530834pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:9530834lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:9530834lld:pubmed