Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:9509140rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0026597lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:9509140lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0040674lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:9509140lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0439830lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:issue7lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:dateCreated1998-3-30lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:abstractTextA study of the effect of the size of a moving target and the extent of its visible motion on motion extrapolation is reported. Targets (a horizontal pair of dots separated by either 0.2 or 0.8 deg) moved across a 10 deg rectilinear path and were then occluded. Observers pressed a key when they thought the leading dot of a hidden target had reached a randomly specified position (0-12 deg from the point of occlusion). In experiment 1, in agreement with velocity-transposition predictions, at moderate (5 deg s-1) and rapid (10 deg s-1) velocities extrapolation times were longer for large targets than for small ones. At slow velocity (2.5 deg s-1) this effect was reversed. In experiment 2 the effect of target size at moderate velocity was found for a short (2.5 deg) visible path. However, the extrapolation time increased with shorter (2.5 deg versus 10 deg) paths. A proposed account of these effects suggests that the visual system performs a spatiotemporal scaling, according to the velocity-transposition principle, not only of visible motion but also of extrapolated motion.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:issn0301-0066lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:authorpubmed-author:CavoniusC RCRlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:authorpubmed-author:SokolovA NANlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:authorpubmed-author:EhrensteinW...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:authorpubmed-author:PavlovaM AMAlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:issnTypePrintlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:volume26lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:pagination875-89lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:dateRevised2006-11-15lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9509140-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9509140-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9509140-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9509140-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9509140-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:9509140-...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:year1997lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:articleTitleMotion extrapolation and velocity transposition.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:affiliationInstitut für Arbeitsphysiologie, Dortmund, Germany.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:9509140pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov'tlld:pubmed