pubmed-article:7496983 | rdf:type | pubmed:Citation | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0395416 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:7496983 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0233849 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:7496983 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0563147 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:7496983 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0679006 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:issue | 4 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:dateCreated | 1996-1-16 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:abstractText | A substantial number of patients who elect to undergo photorefractive keratectomy do so without the motivation of occupational uncorrected vision requirements. We hypothesized that information processing preferences for the auditory (versus visual) modality in a global, associative (versus detailed, sensory-oriented) style with adaptability and risk-taking (versus predictability) personality characteristics would predominate in patients electing photorefractive keratectomy. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:language | eng | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:journal | http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:citationSubset | IM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:status | MEDLINE | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:issn | 1081-597X | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:RyanR ARA | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:AquavellaJ... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:EricksonPP | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:EricksonD BDB | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:issnType | Print | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:volume | 11 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:owner | NLM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:authorsComplete | Y | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:pagination | 267-74; discussion 274-81 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:dateRevised | 2008-11-21 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:7496983-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:articleTitle | Cognitive styles and personality characteristics strongly influence the decision to have photorefractive keratectomy. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:affiliation | Department of Education, Niagara University, NY 14109, USA. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:7496983 | pubmed:publicationType | Journal Article | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:7496983 | lld:pubmed |