pubmed-article:6537252 | rdf:type | pubmed:Citation | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0030362 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:6537252 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0013443 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:6537252 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0442043 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:6537252 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0308269 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:6537252 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C2699488 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:issue | 4 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:dateCreated | 1985-10-1 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:abstractText | On the assumption that temporal resolution underlies an individual's ear advantage for speech perception, one would predict an ear advantage for a temporal resolution task to correlate precisely with an ear advantage for the discrimination of consonant-vowel syllables that differ in their temporal features. A gap detection task that required the resolution of brief silent intervals in bursts of noise was employed to test this hypothesis in four baboons. The findings offer support for such a hypothesis and thus increase the feasibility of an animal model of functional asymmetry in the auditory system. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:grant | http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:grant | http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:language | eng | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:journal | http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:citationSubset | IM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:status | MEDLINE | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:month | Oct | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:issn | 0278-2626 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:PohlPP | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:issnType | Print | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:volume | 3 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:owner | NLM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:authorsComplete | Y | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:pagination | 438-44 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:dateRevised | 2007-11-14 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:6537252-... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:year | 1984 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:articleTitle | Ear advantages for temporal resolution in baboons. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:publicationType | Journal Article | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:publicationType | Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:6537252 | pubmed:publicationType | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:6537252 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:6537252 | lld:pubmed |