Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:21239212rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0031516lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21239212lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0037709lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21239212lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0441472lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21239212lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0728831lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:issue2lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:dateCreated2011-1-31lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:abstractTextAlthough vision seems to predominate in triggering the simulation of the behaviour and mental states of others, the social perception of actions might rely on auditory and olfactory information not only when vision is lacking (e.g. in congenitally blind individuals), but also in daily life (e.g. hearing footsteps along a dark street prompts an appropriate fight-or-fly reaction and smelling the scent of coffee prompts the act of grasping a mug). Here, we review recent evidence showing that non-visual, telereceptor-mediated motor mapping might occur as an autonomous process, as well as within the context of the multimodal perceptions and representations that characterize real-world experiences. Moreover, we discuss the role of auditory and olfactory resonance in anticipating the actions of others and, therefore, in shaping social interactions.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:monthFeblld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:issn1879-307Xlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:authorpubmed-author:AgliotiSalvat...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:authorpubmed-author:PazzagliaMari...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:copyrightInfoCopyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:issnTypeElectroniclld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:volume15lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:pagination47-55lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21239212...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21239212...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21239212...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21239212...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21239212...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21239212...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:21239212...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:year2011lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:articleTitleSounds and scents in (social) action.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:affiliationDipartimento di Psicologia, Sapienza University of Rome, Via dei Marsi 78, Rome I-00185, Italy. salvatoremaria.aglioti@uniroma1.itlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:publicationTypeReviewlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:21239212pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov'tlld:pubmed