Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:11303931rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0008059lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0001675lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0023008lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0030971lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0205307lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0454651lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1705165lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0678587lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C2700061lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1705938lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1527178lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:issue3lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:dateCreated2001-4-16lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:abstractTextThe current investigation studied whether adults, children with normally developing language aged 4-5 years, and children with specific language impairment, aged 5-6 years identified vowels on the basis of steady-state or transitional formant frequencies. Four types of synthetic tokens, created with a female voice, served as stimuli: (1) steady-state centers for the vowels [i] and [ae]; (2) voweless tokens with transitions appropriate for [bib] and [baeb]; (3) "congruent" tokens that combined the first two types of stimuli into [bib] and [baeb]; and (4) "conflicting" tokens that combined the transitions from [bib] with the vowel from [baeb] and vice versa. Results showed that children with language impairment identified the [i] vowel more poorly than other subjects for both the voweless and congruent tokens. Overall, children identified vowels most accurately in steady-state centers and congruent stimuli (ranging between 94%-96%). They identified the vowels on the basis of transitions only from "voweless" tokens with 89% and 83.5% accuracy for the normally developing and language impaired groups, respectively. Children with normally developing language used steady-state cues to identify vowels in 87% of the conflicting stimuli, whereas children with language impairment did so for 79% of the stimuli. Adults were equally accurate for voweless, steady-state, and congruent tokens (ranging between 99% to 100% accuracy) and used both steady-state and transition cues for vowel identification. Results suggest that most listeners prefer the steady state for vowel identification but are capable of using the onglide/offglide transitions for vowel identification. Results were discussed with regard to Nittrouer's developmental weighting shift hypothesis and Strange and Jenkin's dynamic specification theory.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:monthMarlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:issn0001-4966lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:authorpubmed-author:SussmanJ EJElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:issnTypePrintlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:volume109lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:pagination1173-80lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:dateRevised2006-12-27lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11303931...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:year2001lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:articleTitleVowel perception by adults and children with normal language and specific language impairment: based on steady states or transitions?lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:affiliationDepartment of Communicative Disorders and Sciences, University at Buffalo, New York 14214, USA. jsussman@acsu.buffalo.edulld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:publicationTypeComparative Studylld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11303931pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov'tlld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:11303931lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:11303931lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:11303931lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:11303931lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:11303931lld:pubmed