pubmed-article:11302371 | rdf:type | pubmed:Citation | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0035820 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0699810 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0021467 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0036576 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0597931 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0237467 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C0021469 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | lifeskim:mentions | umls-concept:C1948020 | lld:lifeskim |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:issue | 1 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:dateCreated | 2001-4-13 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:abstractText | Switching between tasks leads to response-time (RT) costs at switch points (local switch costs) and often to RT costs at no-switch transitions that occur in the context of a task-switching block (global set-selection costs). With trial-to-trial cuing of tasks, moderate age effects were obtained for local switch costs, but large age effects were obtained for global selection costs. In Experiment 1, set-specific inhibition was found to be at least as large in old as in young adults, thus ruling out an inhibition deficit as a reason for age differences in global costs. In Experiment 2, large age differences in global costs were limited to conditions of ambiguous stimuli and full response-set overlap. This pattern of results suggests a greater reliance on set-updating processes in old than in young adults. The role of these processes is to ensure unambiguos internal control settings when ambiguity arises from stimuli and response specifications. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:language | eng | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:journal | http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:citationSubset | IM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:status | MEDLINE | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:month | Mar | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:issn | 0882-7974 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:author | pubmed-author:MazaCC | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:issnType | Print | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:volume | 16 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:owner | NLM | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:authorsComplete | Y | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:pagination | 96-109 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:dateRevised | 2006-11-15 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:meshHeading | pubmed-meshheading:11302371... | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:year | 2001 | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:articleTitle | Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:affiliation | Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene 97403, USA. mayr@oregon.uoregon.edu | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:publicationType | Journal Article | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:publicationType | Comparative Study | lld:pubmed |
pubmed-article:11302371 | pubmed:publicationType | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |
http://linkedlifedata.com/r... | pubmed:referesTo | pubmed-article:11302371 | lld:pubmed |