Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:11273403rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1555029lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11273403lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1516050lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11273403lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1552603lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11273403lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1706202lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11273403lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0332514lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:issue3lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:dateCreated2001-3-1lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:abstractTextWe report evidence demonstrating that a search asymmetry favoring concave over convex targets can be reversed by altering the figure-ground assignment of edges in shapes. Visual search for a concave target among convex distractors is faster than search for a convex target among concave distractors (a search asymmetry). By using shapes with ambiguous local figure-ground relations, we demonstrated that search can be efficient (with search slopes around 10 ms/item) or inefficient (with search slopes around 30-40 ms/item) with the same stimuli, depending on whether edges are assigned to concave or convex "figures." This assignment process can operate in a top-down manner, according to the task set. The results suggest that attention is allocated to spatial regions following the computation of figure-ground relations in parallel across the elements present. This computation can also be modulated by top-down processes.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:monthMaylld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:issn0956-7976lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:authorpubmed-author:MüllerHHlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:authorpubmed-author:HumphreysG...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:issnTypePrintlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:volume11lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:pagination196-201lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:dateRevised2011-5-20lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11273403...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11273403...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11273403...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11273403...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11273403...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11273403...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:11273403...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:year2000lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:articleTitleA search asymmetry reversed by figure-ground assignment.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:affiliationCognitive Science Research Centre, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, B15 2TT, United Kingdom. g.w.humphreys@bham.ac.uklld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:11273403pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov'tlld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:11273403lld:pubmed