Statements in which the resource exists.
SubjectPredicateObjectContext
pubmed-article:10943701rdf:typepubmed:Citationlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0035820lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:10943701lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0006104lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:10943701lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0152309lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:10943701lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0034754lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:10943701lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0233735lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:10943701lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C1705313lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:10943701lifeskim:mentionsumls-concept:C0441712lld:lifeskim
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:issue11lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:dateCreated2000-11-21lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:abstractTextThe purpose of this study was to test the neurological validity of a dual-route model of reading by asking patients, who were undergoing electrocortical stimulation mapping, to read words with irregular print-to-sound correspondences and pseudowords. Brain activation profiles were also obtained from these patients during an auditory and a visual word recognition task using whole-head magnetic source imaging. We demonstrated that reading is subserved by at least two brain mechanisms that are anatomically dissociable. One mechanism subserves assembled phonology and depends on the activity of the posterior part of the left superior temporal gyrus (STGp), whereas the second is responsible for addressed phonology and does not necessarily involve this region. The contribution of STGp to reading appears to be based on its specialization for phonological analysis operations, involved in the processing of both spoken and written language.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:granthttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:languageenglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:journalhttp://linkedlifedata.com/r...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:citationSubsetIMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:statusMEDLINElld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:monthAuglld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:issn0959-4965lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorpubmed-author:FletcherJ MJMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorpubmed-author:PapanicolaouA...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorpubmed-author:WhelessJ WJWlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorpubmed-author:MaggioW WWWlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorpubmed-author:SimonP JPJlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorpubmed-author:BreierJ IJIlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorpubmed-author:CastilloE MEMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:issnTypePrintlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:day3lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:volume11lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:ownerNLMlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:authorsCompleteYlld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:pagination2443-7lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:dateRevised2007-11-14lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:meshHeadingpubmed-meshheading:10943701...lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:year2000lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:articleTitleBrain mechanisms for reading: the role of the superior temporal gyrus in word and pseudoword naming.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:affiliationVivian L. Smith Center for Neurologic Research, Department of Neurosurgery, The University of Texas-Houston Medical School, 77030, USA.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:publicationTypeJournal Articlelld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S.lld:pubmed
pubmed-article:10943701pubmed:publicationTypeResearch Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S.lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:10943701lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:10943701lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:10943701lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:10943701lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:10943701lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:10943701lld:pubmed
http://linkedlifedata.com/r...pubmed:referesTopubmed-article:10943701lld:pubmed